Home > Cognition > Piaget's Stage (Part 2)
Piaget's Stage (Part 2)
Posted on Friday, May 22, 2009
Third Stage: Concrete Operations
In the concrete operational stage, which cover roughly the age from 7 to 11, the child becomes capable of various logical operations, but only with concrete things. An operation is a type of action-a manipulation of objects or their internal representations. It calls for transforming information so that it can be used more selectively. Operations make trial and error unnecessary because the child can think through the possibilities of certain actions and results of actions.
Operational thought replaces the impressionistic leaps from data to conclusions with a series of small scale, reversible steps, each of which can be judged as reasonable or unreasonable. if information is concrete, comparisons can be made accurately. Thus the child is not taken in by changed beaker shapes as liquid is poured back and forth. He can imagine operations and anticipate results. For example, until this stage is reached, the children can not tell you with any conviction what the other side of the moon is like. But in the stage of concrete operations he can, in his mind, manipulate the moon by turning it around and will tell you that it probably looks just like this side of the moon.
During this stage the child becomes able to handle classification systems like that shown in figure 3. This means that he becomes able to handle complex logical ideas such as:
Fourth Stage: Formal Operations
In the formal operational stage, which covers the ages from about 11 to 14, the student become capable of logical thinking with abstractions, that is, with the "possible" as well as the "here and now". Scientific thinking of hypothetico-deductive type is is now possible. He can draw conclusions, offer interpretations, and develop hypoteses. His thought has become flexible and powerful. He can:
The developmental process
How do the children behave so as to acquire these successive intellectual capabilities? In general, they engage in a process of seeking an equilibrium between what they presently perceive, know, and understand, on the one hand, and what they see in any new phenomenon, experience, or problem. If their present condition can handle the new situation, their equilibrium is undisturbed. If it cannot, then some intellectual is necessary to restore the equilibrium. That is, some adaptation by the organism to its environment must take place.
Adaptation takes two forms, which occur simultaneously: assimilation and accommodation. Assimilation is the process of changing what is perceived so that it fits present cognitive structures, while accommodation is the process of changing the cognitive structures so that they fit what is perceived.
Assimilation is comparable to the acts of chewing and digesting in order to transform food into something the body can use. Assimilation transforms new ideas into something that fits into one’s cognitive structure. Accommodation is comparable to the adjustments made by the body in eating and using food. Such as opening the mouth, contracting the muscles of the throat, esophagus, and stomach, and secreting the digestive juices. In the same way, an existing cognitive structure must be modified, extended, or refined in coming to grips with a new or anomalous idea.
Assimilation and accommodation may be hard to tell apart. They go on simultaneously and, in the processing of ideas, they are two sides of the same coin, the process of maintaining equilibrium between the person and his environment. When we interpret, construe, and structure, we are changing the nature of reality to make it fit into our own cognitive structure. This is assimilation. When we adjust our own ideas, as in calling up memory, a similarity, or analogy, in order to make sense of reality, we are accommodating.
The processes of assimilation and accommodation have somewhat lasting effects on the cognitive structures involved. As ways by which an organism adapts to its cognitive environment, they are comparable to the biological process of adaptation that affect the structure and functions of an organism’s body. So a child’s cognitive schema, that is, his cognitive organization and structure, gradually changes as a function of his experience. As children mature, they go through the four stage of cognitive development described above.
Reference:
Educational Psychology. Gage, N.L & Berliner, David C.
In the concrete operational stage, which cover roughly the age from 7 to 11, the child becomes capable of various logical operations, but only with concrete things. An operation is a type of action-a manipulation of objects or their internal representations. It calls for transforming information so that it can be used more selectively. Operations make trial and error unnecessary because the child can think through the possibilities of certain actions and results of actions.
Operational thought replaces the impressionistic leaps from data to conclusions with a series of small scale, reversible steps, each of which can be judged as reasonable or unreasonable. if information is concrete, comparisons can be made accurately. Thus the child is not taken in by changed beaker shapes as liquid is poured back and forth. He can imagine operations and anticipate results. For example, until this stage is reached, the children can not tell you with any conviction what the other side of the moon is like. But in the stage of concrete operations he can, in his mind, manipulate the moon by turning it around and will tell you that it probably looks just like this side of the moon.
During this stage the child becomes able to handle classification systems like that shown in figure 3. This means that he becomes able to handle complex logical ideas such as:
- composition, that is the idea that, whenever two elements of a system are combined (for example reciprocating-engine autos, A, and other autos, A'), we obtain other element of system (that is, autos, B). Or if we combine autos (B) and other means of transportation (B'), we obtain means of transportation (C).
- associativity, the idea that the sum is independent of the order in which things are added. Thus in the example above, A + A' = B, and A' + A = B.
- reversibility, the idea that not only can we add nonautos (B') with autos (B) to obtain means of transportation (C), but when we subtract nonautos from modes of transportation, we obtain autos.
Figure 3
Classification Scheme showing kinds of operations that child can handle in the stage of concrete operations
Classification Scheme showing kinds of operations that child can handle in the stage of concrete operations
Fourth Stage: Formal Operations
In the formal operational stage, which covers the ages from about 11 to 14, the student become capable of logical thinking with abstractions, that is, with the "possible" as well as the "here and now". Scientific thinking of hypothetico-deductive type is is now possible. He can draw conclusions, offer interpretations, and develop hypoteses. His thought has become flexible and powerful. He can:
- work out all the logical possibilities, without needing to determine which ones actually occur in the real world. This capability refers not mere imagination, since younger children can engage in fanciful thinking, but to lawful and systematic exposition of logical alternatives. This means also that the student can
- conduct a combinational analysis of possibilities. Thus, given two possible causes, C1 and C2, and a result, r, the student can formulate the possibilities that
either C1 or C2 causes r,
both C1 or C2 causes r,
neither C1 or C2 causes r,
C1 could cause r, but C2 could not,
C2 could cause r, but C1 could not,
C1 and C2 could cause r, but neither one alone could,
C1 could cause r, but only if C2 is absent,
and so on
- do propositional thinking. Thus the adolescent can take propositions of the kind just stated about C1, C2, and r, and regard them as elements to be combined into new, higher order propositions, such as "either p1 or p2 can be true, but not both."
- generalize from propositions based on one kind of content, say, clay or beads, to many other kinds of content, such as water, wood pieces, checkers, physical objects in general, all liquids, and all numerically denotable quantities.
- Subject: (Tries 4 x g, then 2 x g, and then 3 x g). I think I did everything. I tried them all.
Experimenter: What else could you have done?
Subject: I don't know
Experimenter: (Give the glasses to the subject again)
Subject: (Repeats 1 x g, 2 x g, etc)
Experimenter: You took each bottle separately. What else could you have done?
Subject: Take two bottle at the same time. (Subject then tries 1 x 4 x g, then 2 x 3 x g, failing to cross over between the two sets of bottle: for example, 1 x 2, 1 x 3, 2 x 4, and 3 x 4, etc)
Experimenter: (Suggests that subject add other bottles)
Subject: (Puts 1 x g in the glass already containing 2 x 3, which results in the appearance of color)
Experimenter: Try to make the color again
Subject: Do I put in 2 or 3? (Subject tries with 2 x 4 x g, then adds 3, then tries it with 1 x 4 x 2 x g) I don't remember anymore.
- Subject: You have to try it with all the bottles. I'll begin with the one at the end. (Subject goes from 1 to 4 with g). It doesn't work anymore. Maybe I have to mix them. (Subject tries 1 x 2 x g, then 1 x 3 x g). it turn yellow.
Experimenter: Are there other solutions?
Subject: I'll try. (Subject tries all six combinations of flasks, taking two at a time). It doesn't work. It only works with 1 x 3 x g.
Experimenter: Yes, and what about 2 and 4?
Subject: 2 and 4 don't make any color together. They are negative. perhaps I could add 4 in 1 x 3 x g to see if it would cancel out the color. (The subject pours 4 into the yellow mixture). Liquid 4 cancel it all. I'll have to see if 2 has the same influence. (He pours 2 into mixture of 1 x 3 x g). No, it doesn't, so 2 and 4 are not alike because acts on 1 x 3 and 2 doesn't.
Experimenter: What is there in 2 and 4?
Subject: In 4 certainly water. No the opposite, in 2 certainly water since it didn't act on liquids. That makes things clearer.
Experimenter: And if I were to tell you that 4 is water?
Subject: If the liquid 4 is water, when you put it into 1 x 3 it wouldn't completely prevent the yellow from forming. It isn't water. It's something harmful.
The developmental process
How do the children behave so as to acquire these successive intellectual capabilities? In general, they engage in a process of seeking an equilibrium between what they presently perceive, know, and understand, on the one hand, and what they see in any new phenomenon, experience, or problem. If their present condition can handle the new situation, their equilibrium is undisturbed. If it cannot, then some intellectual is necessary to restore the equilibrium. That is, some adaptation by the organism to its environment must take place.
Adaptation takes two forms, which occur simultaneously: assimilation and accommodation. Assimilation is the process of changing what is perceived so that it fits present cognitive structures, while accommodation is the process of changing the cognitive structures so that they fit what is perceived.
Assimilation is comparable to the acts of chewing and digesting in order to transform food into something the body can use. Assimilation transforms new ideas into something that fits into one’s cognitive structure. Accommodation is comparable to the adjustments made by the body in eating and using food. Such as opening the mouth, contracting the muscles of the throat, esophagus, and stomach, and secreting the digestive juices. In the same way, an existing cognitive structure must be modified, extended, or refined in coming to grips with a new or anomalous idea.
Assimilation and accommodation may be hard to tell apart. They go on simultaneously and, in the processing of ideas, they are two sides of the same coin, the process of maintaining equilibrium between the person and his environment. When we interpret, construe, and structure, we are changing the nature of reality to make it fit into our own cognitive structure. This is assimilation. When we adjust our own ideas, as in calling up memory, a similarity, or analogy, in order to make sense of reality, we are accommodating.
The processes of assimilation and accommodation have somewhat lasting effects on the cognitive structures involved. As ways by which an organism adapts to its cognitive environment, they are comparable to the biological process of adaptation that affect the structure and functions of an organism’s body. So a child’s cognitive schema, that is, his cognitive organization and structure, gradually changes as a function of his experience. As children mature, they go through the four stage of cognitive development described above.
Reference:
Educational Psychology. Gage, N.L & Berliner, David C.
Category Article Cognition
Come from blogwalking
nice post, thanks
NS:
U comment I follow
hi nice blog thank you...
I learn a lot from your blog. This is a well explain behavioral and psychological attitude and trait of a child. And it answered some of my question on why kids act differently.
good completion of part one .. thank you
very good post, i can see this post is very thorough and well thought out.